Love and Co-Sleeping in Childhood

PDF Version.

Sharing a bed with another individual is commonly associated with love and loving. Like other animals that live in groups, many humans sleep together, although this can vary based on culture and age. In many cultures, co-sleeping, or sharing a bed with a family member, is normal. Why so?

Sleeping Together Among Animals

This way of sleeping together is found even among animals. Recent studies on animal sleep behavior have demonstrated that sleep is affected by the presence of surrounding animals. For example, olive baboons exhibit reduced sleep duration with larger group sizes, whereas mice are capable of synchronizing their rapid eye movement cycles (Chakravarty et al., 2024).

The Cultural Practices of Co-Sleeping Across Cultures

Sleeping together and bedsharing are common cultural norms, with some variations across cultures and at different stages of the life cycle (Andre et al., 2021; Mindell et al., 2010).

Co-sleeping between caregivers and infants is prevalent across many societies, with rates reaching 60–100% in certain regions of South America, Asia, and Africa, and smaller rates of co-sleeping in Western societies. In western societies, many people expect to sleep alone, with a romantic partner, or with a spouse (Mindell et al., 2010).

Developmental Benefits of Co-Sleeping in Childhood

Evolutionary scientists believe that throughout human history, co-sleeping has been important for keeping babies warm and safe, the crucial factors for their survival (Barry, 2021).

Many cultures view night awakenings as a normal aspect of breastfeeding and development, and they do not expect babies to self-soothe when they wake up (D’Souza & Cassels, 2023; Luijk et al., 2013). Some researchers found that co-sleeping benefits infants’ emotional development, promotes parent-child bonding, and aids infants’ stress hormone regulation (Barry, 2019; Beijers et al., 2013).

Modern Practices of Co-Sleeping

Nowadays, according to global surveys, co-sleeping during childhood beyond infancy is also relatively prevalent. A survey of more than 7,000 families administered in the UK in 2010 revealed that 6% of children consistently shared a bed with someone until at least four years of age (Blair et al., 2010; Mindell et al., 2013; 2014).

Some families engage in co-sleeping as a remedy for their child’s sleep difficulties. In many countries, including certain Western nations such as Sweden, child-parent bedsharing is culturally perceived as an integral aspect of a nurturing environment. Children frequently co-sleep with their parents until they reach school age (Marakovitz et al., 2023; Welles‐Nystrom, 2005).

It is also prevalent for siblings to share a room or even a bed. A 2021 US study revealed that more than 36% of young children aged three to five years engaged in bedsharing overnight, whether with caregivers, siblings, pets, or a combination thereof. Co-sleeping diminishes yet persists among older children, with up to 13.8% of co-sleeping parents in Australia, the UK, and other nations indicating that their child was aged between five and twelve years during co-sleeping occurrences (Mason et al., 2021; D’Souza et al., 2023).

The Benefits and Downsides of Co-Sleeping

The advantages and disadvantages of co-sleeping may vary in children with conditions such as autism spectrum disorders, mental health disorders, and chronic illnesses. These children may encounter increased anxiety, sensory sensitivities, and physical discomfort that hinder their ability to fall and remain asleep. In these cases, co-sleeping can offer them reassurance (Jacquier & Newman, 2017; Köse et al., 2017; Palmer et al., 2018; Sidhoum et al., 2019).

Although common, infant co-sleeping remains a contentious issue. Some Western viewpoints, which prioritize self-reliance, contend that solitary sleeping facilitates self-soothing in infants during nocturnal awakenings. So, it is still unclear whether co-sleeping or solitary sleeping is more beneficial in the modern world of cultural diversity.

References

Andre, C. J., Lovallo, V., & Spencer, R. M. (2021). The effects of bed sharing on sleep: From partners to petsSleep Health7(3), 314-323.

Barry, E. S. (2019). Co-sleeping as a proximal context for infant development: The importance of physical touchInfant Behavior and Development57, 101385.

Barry, E. S. (2021). Sleep consolidation, sleep problems, and co-sleeping: rethinking normal infant sleep as species-typicalThe Journal of Genetic Psychology182(4), 183-204.

Beijers, R., Riksen-Walraven, J. M., & de Weerth, C. (2013). Cortisol regulation in 12-month-old human infants: associations with the infants’ early history of breastfeeding and co-sleepingStress16(3), 267-277.

Blair, P. S., Heron, J., & Fleming, P. J. (2010). Relationship between bed sharing and breastfeeding: longitudinal, population-based analysisPediatrics126(5), e1119-e1126.

Chakravarty, P., Ashbury, A. M., Strandburg-Peshkin, A., Iffelsberger, J., Goldshtein, A., Schuppli, C., … & Crofoot, M. C. (2024). The sociality of sleep in animal groupsTrends in Ecology & Evolution.

D’Souza, L., & Cassels, T. (2023). Contextual considerations in infant sleep: Offering alternative interventions to families. Sleep Health9(5), 618-625.

D’Souza, L., Morris, Z. A., Borgkvist, A., & Blunden, S. (2023). Understanding motivations and satisfaction with sleep location among co‐sleeping (including bed‐sharing) parents. Family Relations73(2), 661-682.

Jacquier, D., & Newman, C. J. (2017). Co‐sleeping in school‐aged children with a motor disability: a comparative population‐based study. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology59(4), 420-426.

Köse, S., Yılmaz, H., Ocakoğlu, F. T., & Özbaran, N. B. (2017). Sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability without autism spectrum disorder. Sleep Medicine40, 69-77.

Marakovitz, S. E., Sheldrick, R. C., Copeland, W. E., Restrepo, B., Hastedt, I., Carpenter, K. L., … & Egger, H. L. (2023). Associations of preschool reactive bed-sharing with sociodemographic factors, sleep disturbance, and psychopathologyChild and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health17(1), 62.

Mason, G. M., Holmes, J. F., Andre, C., & Spencer, R. M. (2021). Bedsharing in early childhood: frequency, partner characteristics, and relations to sleep. The Journal of Genetic Psychology182(4), 269-288.

Mindell, J. A., Sadeh, A., Wiegand, B., How, T. H., & Goh, D. Y. (2010). Cross-cultural differences in infant and toddler sleep. Sleep Medicine11(3), 274-280.

Mindell, J. A., Sadeh, A., Kwon, R., & Goh, D. Y. (2013). Cross-cultural differences in the sleep of preschool childrenSleep Medicine14(12), 1283-1289.

Mindell, J. A., Sadeh, A., Kwon, R., & Goh, D. Y. T. (2014). “Cross-cultural differences in the sleep of preschool children”: CorrigendumSleep Medicine, 15(12), 1595–1596.

Luijk, M. P., Mileva-Seitz, V. R., Jansen, P. W., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Jaddoe, V. W., Raat, H., … & Tiemeier, H. (2013). Ethnic differences in prevalence and determinants of mother–child bed-sharing in early childhoodSleep Medicine14(11), 1092-1099.

Palmer, C. A., Clementi, M. A., Meers, J. M., & Alfano, C. A. (2018). Co-sleeping among school-aged anxious and non-anxious children: associations with sleep variability and timingJournal of Abnormal Child Psychology46, 1321-1332.

Sidhoum, L., Amaddeo, A., Arroyo, J. O., De Sanctis, L., Khirani, S., & Fauroux, B. (2019). Parent–child co-sleeping in children with co-morbid conditions and sleep-disordered breathingSleep and Breathing23, 327-332.

Welles‐Nystrom, B. (2005). Co‐sleeping as a window into Swedish culture: considerations of gender and health care. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences19(4), 354-360.